The role of functional load in sound change

Languages are constantly changing. Just as words drift in meaning over many generations, the basic sounds of a language can shift over time.

These changes are often highly regular and systematic. For example, during the evolution of Proto-Indo-European (PIE) to Proto-Germanic, there was a series of changes to stop consonants known as Grimm’s Law. The phoneme /d/ (as in dog) shifted to /t/ (as in talk); /t/, meanwhile, became /θ/ (as in think).

These changes weren’t attested in other evolved variants of PIE. This allows us to compare words with common ancestries across language families. The Latin decem has its counterpart in the English ten (Dutch: tien; Norwegian: ti), and the Latin tres (starting with /t/) is a cousin to the English three (starting with /θ/). (Note that many other changes have occurred, which is why you’ll see more differences than simply a shift in /t/ and /d/.)

Grimm’s Law also involved many other changes, and is far from the only major phonological change to occur in the history of Germanic languages (see The Great Vowel Shift). However, Grimm’s Law is an important example of sound change being a regular process–i.e., a given change should apply throughout the lexicon of a language, provided the appropriate conditions are met.

Phonological change is an incredibly rich field of study. Research questions abound, including: the link between perceptual/articulatory constraints and large-scale sound change (Ohala, 1993; Ohala, 1989), the social variables involved (Labov, 1963; Labov, 1986), and more.

For me, one particularly interesting aspect of sound change is its relationship to the creation of homophones: two words that sound the same, but with different meanings (e.g., night and knight).

Sound change and homophony

Sound changes are often classified into one of several categories: mergers, splits, and losses.

A merger occurs when the distinction between two phonemes in a language is lost. An example in English is the famous cot-caught merger. In some dialects of English, the vowel in the word cot is produced (and perceived) differently than the vowel in the caught. However, many dialects of North American English don’t really distinguish between the two. I certainly pronounce them the same way. In other words, the lost of this contrast has produced a homophone.

Other examples of homophony-inducing sound changes in English abound. Modern English witnessed the loss of many word-initial consonant clusters, such as /kn/ and /gn/. In some cases, this created homophones among formerly distinct word pairs, e.g., knight/night, not/knot, and nit/knit. (Interestingly, these word-initial consonant clusters have been fossilized in the spelling of the words.)

Homophony-inducing sound changes aren’t limited to English. Sampson (2013, 2015) describes a series of sound changes that occurred between Middle and Modern Chinese, such as the merging of final -m and final -n as simply -n. This created many homophones, particularly among monosyllabic words 1.

Intuitively, both mergers and losses appear more likely to increase, rather than decrease, the number of homophones in a language, given that they eliminate phoneme contrasts that previously might have distinguished words.

Of course, some kinds of sound change, such as splits could theoretically reduce the number of homophones by increasing the possible phonemic contrasts in a language. The presumed regularity of sound change makes this scenario unlikely: if two words have the same form (e.g., bank and bank), it is unlikely that only one of those words will undergo a sound change, leaving the other unchanged. Still, it is in principle possible, especially if the two words vary considerably in frequency—in which case the more frequent word (e.g., time) tends to undergo a reduction process relative to the less frequent word (e.g., thyme) (Gahl, 2008), perhaps leaving it open to more drastic phonological changes in the future.

The Functional Load Hypothesis

While sound changes clearly can create homophones, some (Martinet, 1952) have argued that a pressure for communicative clarity places an upper-bound on this process. According to the functional load hypothesis, phoneme contrasts that do more “work”—i.e., distinguish more words in a lexicon—should be less likely to merge (or be lost altogether).

This idea makes intuitive sense. If we conceive of language change as an evolutionary process subject to communicative pressures (e.g., clarity), then changes that raise opportunities for misunderstanding might be selected against. Under this scenario, sound changes that create homophones should be less likely to occur than those that don’t.

For example, in General American English, the onset phonemes /b/ and /p/ distinguish a variety of words. To name just a few monosyllabic pairs: pat/bat, pot/bot, pit/bit

Unfortunately, the idea is also challenging to test. And as Sampson (2013) points out, it’s unclear exactly what the hypothesis means. There are many attested cases of sound changes creating homophones (such as the ones above), so it can’t simply mean that sound changes never create homophones.

Testing the hypothesis

Discussion

Future work:

References

Ceolin, A. (2020) Neutral Models of Sound Change.

Gahl, S. (2008). Time and thyme are not homophones: The effect of lemma frequency on word durations in spontaneous speech. Language, 84(3), 474-496.

Ohala, J. J. (1989). Sound change is drawn from a pool of synchronic variation. Language change: Contributions to the study of its causes, 173-198.

Ohala, J. J. (1993). Sound change as nature’s speech perception experiment. Speech Communication, 13(1-2), 155-161.

Labov, W. (1963). The social motivation of a sound change. Word, 19(3), 273-309.

Labov, W. (1986). The social origins of sound change. In Dialect and language variation (pp. 524-541). Academic Press.

Martinet, A. (1952). Function, structure, and sound change. Word, 8(1), 1-32.

Sampson, G. (2015). A Chinese phonological enigma. Journal of Chinese Linguistics, 43(2), 679-691.

Sampson, G. (2015). A Chinese phonological enigma. Journal of Chinese Linguistics, 43(2), 679-691.

Footnotes

  1. According to Sampson (2013), speakers of the language compensated for this increase in lexical ambiguity via the disyllabification of synonyms. For example, “friend” in Mandarin is péngyǒu; in Old Chinese, péng and yǒu were separate words, both meaning “friend”. After the mergers, the morpheme péng was “seven ways ambiguous” (Sampson, 2013, pg. 9). The compound péngyǒu, on the other hand, is unambiguous. 

Written on February 4, 2020